Color Vision
Normal-sighted individuals have three different types of cones that mediate color vision. Each of these cone types is maximally sensitive to a slightly different wavelength of light.
trichromatic theory of color vision
According to the Young-Helmholtz trichromatic theory of color vision, shown in Figure 1, all colors in the spectrum can be produced by combining red, green, and blue. The three types of cones are each receptive to one of the colors.

Colorblindness: A Story
William, a single father, was preparing for a public event when his 7-year-old daughter told him his clothes didn’t match. Concerned, they sought a second opinion from a nearby convenience store. The store clerk examined William’s attire—a bright green pair of pants, a reddish-orange shirt, and a brown tie—and confirmed, “Your clothes definitely don’t match.”
Prompted by these comments, William consulted friends and coworkers, who diplomatically described his style as “unique.” Realizing something might be off, he visited an eye doctor and discovered he was colorblind, unable to distinguish between certain shades of greens, browns, and reds.

Aside from occasional fashion missteps, William’s colorblindness hasn’t significantly affected his daily life. This case highlights how sensory limitations can go unnoticed until a specific situation brings them to light.
Some forms of color deficiency are rare. Seeing in grayscale (only shades of black and white) is extremely rare, and people who do so only have rods, which means they have very low visual acuity and cannot see very well. The most common X-linked inherited abnormality is red-green color blindness (Birch, 2012). Approximately 8% of males with European Caucasian decent, 5% of Asian males, 4% of African males, and less than 2% of indigenous American males, Australian males, and Polynesian males have red-green color deficiency (Birch, 2012). Comparatively, only about 0.4% of females of European Caucasian descent have red-green color deficiency (Birch, 2012).
The trichromatic theory of color vision is not the only theory—another major theory of color vision is known as the opponent-process theory.
opponent-process theory
According to opponent-process theory, color is coded in opponent pairs: black-white, yellow-blue, and green-red. The basic idea is that some cells of the visual system are excited by one of the opponent colors and inhibited by the other. So, a cell that was excited by wavelengths associated with green would be inhibited by wavelengths associated with red, and vice versa.
One of the implications of opponent processing is that we do not experience greenish-reds or yellowish-blues as colors. Another implication is that this leads to the experience of negative afterimages. An afterimage describes the continuation of a visual sensation after removal of the stimulus. For example, when you stare briefly at the sun and then look away from it, you may still perceive a spot of light although the stimulus (the sun) has been removed. When color is involved in the stimulus, the color pairings identified in the opponent-process theory lead to a negative afterimage. You can test this concept using the flag in Figure 3.

But these two theories—the trichromatic theory of color vision and the opponent-process theory—are not mutually exclusive. Research has shown that they just apply to different levels of the nervous system. For visual processing on the retina, trichromatic theory applies: the cones are responsive to three different wavelengths that represent red, blue, and green. But once the signal moves past the retina on its way to the brain, the cells respond in a way consistent with opponent-process theory (Land, 1959; Kaiser, 1997).